Of Mind, Brain, and Politics

1. Introduction

Have you ever heard of the term—“neuropolitics”? I know; it sounds like I just made it up. But in fact, neuropolitics, or otherwise known as political neuroscience, is an emerging field of study at the forefront of political science and social cognition neuroscience that has become popular in the last two decades.

The poet JJ Bola once said in an interview, “Our existence is political — the choices that we make, where we decide to eat, where we buy our clothes, what school we go to, what languages we speak and where we speak them, how we speak…” (1) and I couldn't agree more. Politics is the play of power and influence on decision-making (2), and it does, in fact, play a role in everything that we do in life. Our behavior as a human, a creature of society, is deeply entangled in our own political reality. As an aspiring neuroscientist, I find this very interesting. How is it that the intricate connectivities of billions of tiny cells in our brain and their activities translate to opinions and beliefs that result in collective behavior out in society? Further, how is it influenced by social interactions and societal events?

Historically, there always has been deep philosophical opposition to the union of politics and the brain. The early philosophers like Aristotle, Plato, and Locke embraced dualism—the idea that the mind and the body are two distinct entities (3). This prevented, to an extent, the foray into exploring the material basis of the mind. However, over the last 70 or so years, the field of neuroscience has grown in leaps and bounds. Our understanding of the brain has reached levels unforeseen. With it has come new insight into decision making, belief formation, social behavior (4–6). All of which provide us with an opportunity to examine the connection between politics and the brain. In this essay, I shall attempt to discuss some of the current progress in neuropolitics and the future of this emerging field.

2. Politics on my Mind

Born from the intersection of political psychology, sociobiology, and cognitive neuroscience, the field of neuropolitics aims to study how political information is processed in the brain and is used to make decisions (7). One of the early experiments in this field was a simple one. The researchers presented faces of prominent personalities such as Adolf Hitler and Richard Nixon in split-brain patients to assess the difference in response to political stimulus between the two sides of the brain (8). Based on their results, they proposed that each hemisphere can independently code at least some degree of preference towards political figures. The study was not perfect. It lacked nuance and made many logical jumps, but it was a start.

The actual process of consolidating this field began in the early 2000s when scholars from many academic fields started to recognize the potential of applying understanding from cognitive neuroscience to the study of politics. An idea emerged that political and social cognition are deeply intertwined concepts, and understanding one could dramatically improve the understanding of the other (9–12). Researchers in social cognition such as Darren Schrieber, John Jost, and John T Cacioppo were some of the significant proponents of this idea. Schreiber went on further to make evolutionary arguments for the idea that human brains are “built for politics” based on neural studies of humans and primates (13). 

This concept has been dubbed the “Machiavellian Intelligence” or the “social brain” hypothesis after Machiavelli, the rather infamous author of “The Prince” (14). Nicholas Humphrey, a British neuropsychologist, once explained the gist of this argument with a straightforward example. In Defoe’s masterpiece, Robinson Crusoe’s survival on the desert island was technically challenging. But, the really hard problems come from the arrival of Man Friday (15). In essence, social interactions require the most complex form of information processing. The same might have been the primary driving force behind the expanding human behavioral repertoire and evolution of the complex brain we see today. After the initial conception of these ideas, they have been elaborated on by a multitude of researchers from diverse backgrounds in the past years (16). 

With advances in neuroimaging methods like EEG, structural and functional MRI, it has become a lot more accessible to look at brain connections and activity during a large number of tasks (17). Insight into the function of circuits in the brain involved in decision-making, reward processing, and social cognition would not have been possible without these methods. They have made it possible to examine the neural basis of political cognition. Ingrid Haas, a professor of neuropolitics at the University of Nebraska-Lincoln, broadly divides the study of the interaction of the brain and politics into three major categories: (a) the neural basis of political ideology (b) understanding how people evaluate other people in a political context, and lastly (c) neural factors that influence one’s political participation (7). Let’s take a look at them one by one.

2. The Neural Basis of Political Ideology

Social creatures as we are, Humans have a tendency to associate ourselves with categorized communities of people with similar beliefs (18). In the political world, no categorization is considered more influential than the apparent dichotomy of Liberals and Conservatives. Would not understanding its basis be the holy grail of Neuropolitics? But of course, it is one of the most prominent and controversial questions in this field. Many studies have attempted to find biological differences between liberal and conservatives. However, the results are weak and often contradictory.

A difference that has been seen between the two political groups is the relative size and the activity of three brain regions—the ACC, the insula, and the amygdala. Multiple studies have reported that the ACC and insula, areas of the brain involved in detecting and processing conflicting information or beliefs, are larger and more sensitive in Liberals than conservatives (19–22). This increased sensitivity has been further associated with a preference for social equality and social change (22). On the other hand, studies show that conservatives have a larger and more active amygdala, the brain’s emotion and motivation center (19). This has further been linked to a tendency to justify the status quo (23–26). A recent study has found that anterior insular activation is linked to the tendency of conservatives to classify Black and White mixed-race individuals as Black (27). Another study found that the brains of conservatives and liberals have highly polarised responses in the DLPFC in response to discussions of immigration policies and is supported by recent fMRI, EEG and lesion studies. (28–31).

One fascinating study done by the Schreiber Lab in 2013 found that in a task that assesses risk-taking behavior, Liberal-leaning Democrats and Conservative-leaning Republicans seem to perform similarly when it comes to the outcome. However, the activity in their brains as they make decisions is different, with conservatives having more amygdala activity and liberals having more insular activity (32). This led to an exciting hypothesis that even when conservatives and liberals make the same decision, the thought process behind them is dramatically different.  Other studies have tried to link general ideological principles to activity in the brain. For example, studies have found that insular activation is linked to egalitarian beliefs (33), the opposite response is associated with right-wing authoritarianism (34), and greater connectivity between amygdala and BNST relates to economic conservatism (35). 

But at the same time, many attempts to replicate similar results have been met with failures (22,36). Two recent studies found no evidence of a direct link between brain activity and self-reported liberalism/conservatism (37,38). They suggest that there might be a dependence on other social and political factors which may not have been considered earlier such as age and geopolitical context. As most such studies have come out of the US and have not been explored thoroughly in the international context, the concern is valid.

While these results are pretty interesting, one has to be careful with interpretation because it can be a really slippery slope. Being a conservative does not mean you are inherently more emotional; Being a liberal does not make you a genius logician. Most current results in this field are correlations, and the causality hasn’t been tested. What does that mean? It means that understanding the neural basis of liberalism and conservatism is right now a “chicken and egg problem” (39). Neither the structure nor the behavior of the tiny neurons in the brain is absolute; both change with time and experience (40). With short-term experiments and the current technology available, it is impossible to say whether the brain’s innate structure led to a person’s political ideology or whether it is the person’s experiences over their lifetimes that made their brain look and behave that way.

3. Interpersonal Evaluation in a Political Context

The most common question that political neuroscientists face from the public is often a very straightforward question. Can you tell who people will vote for from their brain activity? While the ethics of being able to do that is somewhat questionable, it is a rather intriguing question. In a 2006 study, Spezio and colleagues performed a simple experiment. They simulated a ‘mini election’ where the participants would be presented with just the images of two unfamiliar politicians in rapid succession and asked to vote for one of them (41). When looking at the candidate they did not vote for, the participant’s ACC and insula showed more significant activity associated with a feeling of a personal threat. Another study suggested that looking at preferred candidates triggers activity in the brain’s reward processing center, the ventral striatum, indicating that people see them as ‘rewarding’ (42).

Now the big question is: Are these rapid, reflexive responses actually reflective of the outcome of actual elections? There is evidence to suggest so (43,44). A study published in PNAS showed that participant’s beliefs after rapid exposure to images of US gubernatorial candidates were enough to predict the actual election outcomes (43). This prediction was actually better than when the participants were allowed to think and decide. Surprising, isn’t it? Guess what’s more. One would expect that they would need to have the conscious memory of the candidates’ policies to choose a candidate aligned with their own ideologies, right? Researchers have found that patients suffering from damage in the brain’s memory centers which caused anterograde amnesia could choose a candidate with the same political-leaning as them, despite severe memory impairment (45). They did this as accurately as neurologically unaffected people. This suggested that the conscious memory of policy might be inconsequential to voting choices. 

If that is so, what is the driving factor for voting choices? Many political scientists have suggested that partisanship can be one such factor. Some neurological studies have found that exposure to a candidate from a different party triggers activity in the brain’s circuits that have been associated with cognitive control of negative emotions (42,46). This has been suggested to provide a possible neural basis for the “us vs. them” mentality. Another fascinating experiment discovered that when participants are faced with information that is incongruent with their own beliefs, people who are more resistant to the counterarguments show more robust activation in their emotion centers (47–49). This suggests that the political decisions of a person might be constrained by their emotional response.

Often researchers in this field have a tendency to focus on the politics of the state, i.e., the politics concerning voting, elections, and government policies. However, neuropolitics can also provide us further insight into understanding other interpersonal, political evaluations such as racial and ethnic attitudes (50,51). In fact, these studies were at the forefront of the field when the transition from cognitive neuroscience to social/political cognition first began (52). 

Early studies by Elizabeth Phelps and Matthew Leiberman explored the underlying neural substrates that are involved in the evaluation of Black individuals by Caucasian participants (53,54). They found that amygdala activation correlated with the unconscious (implicit) but not the conscious (explicit) racial attitudes. Another study showed that exposure to Black faces in a pro-White biased individual has higher activation of the ACC and DLPFC than a person with less pro-White bias (55). ACC and DLPFC are both centers for conflict detection and modulation. This suggests a conflict of conscious egalitarian principles and subconscious pro-White biases. Building on this further, it was shown that people who prefer racial equality without external motivation had a more robust response in the ACC compared to their counterparts (56,57). 

A prevalent racial stereotype is that people find individuals from a different race to look the same.  Studies of the Fusiform Gyrus have shown that the FFA, a region implicated in face recognition, seems to respond stronger to faces of the same race than other races (58,59). This likely results in the homogenization of other race’s individual identity. Together, the FFA, Amygdala, ACC show how diverse racial attitudes might be reflected in the brain (60). It still remains to be discovered how the variation between the structure and behavior of these brain regions might influence actual decision-making in the real world. These results are not limited to just racial identity.

Chimpanzees often have large communal conflicts and annex territory driven by a biological drive towards a tribal organization (14). Famous author Robert Sapolsky has hypothesized that humans share this innate tendency to form strong associations with people close to them (61). This simultaneously drives conflict against the out-groups, possibly because of the activation of the same pathways as race. He then builds this idea to propose a neural theory of nationalism (62). Some other recent studies have discovered other brain regions, such as the PCC and OFC, where a greater cell density is associated with nationalism (63). This was also linked to less compassion and a feeling of superiority over others. The study even managed to distinguish between neural correlates of patriotism and nationalism. It is again important to note that almost all of these results are correlational and do not prove a causal effect. They are still educated guesses about how things might be. 

4. The Drive behind Political Participation

Research in neuropolitics has also tried answering an even more fundamental question—why do people even choose to participate in political decision-making? (7) Theories of social cognition like Schieber’s theory of a political brain (discussed earlier) are but a few of the attempts to answer this question (13).  Some studies even have tried and found genetic correlates of voter turnout (64,65). While these studies themselves might have many caveats, the number of citations they have received gives an idea about the interest in this question. 

Often asking such a broad ‘why’ question is a vast undertaking requiring insight that is not available yet. In such a scenario, maybe it is better to focus on a more specific question: What drives a person to vote during elections? Psychologists have long proposed that if the process of voting is rewarding to the voter, it increases the likelihood that they will end up voting (66). Is that really true? The answer to this question is likely linked to political interest. A study showed that people with a higher interest in politics have greater activation of the brains’ reward and emotion centers in response to political statements they agree with (67). This suggested that for some people, political participation itself might be a source of utility. 

Not all people vote with the same motives. Another study has found that the nature of the reward to the voter might also vary between people. They found that people with less altruistic tendencies have an activation of the reward pathway linked to immediate utility rather than a long-term expectation of utility under an uncertain scenario (68). There are many other ways of political participation such as protesting, making political donations, and even becoming a politician, the neural basis of which are yet to be explored. 

5. Conclusion

Neuropolitics is fascinating indeed, but with the excitement and grand nature of the questions asked, there comes a tendency to over-interpret results, make logical jumps without enough justification or evidence, and suggest causal relationships with raw correlational data. Sometimes the interpretations have, in fact, gone too far, triggering neuroscientists to distance themselves from the studies (69,70). Critics have further pointed out that “Neuroscience is just not there yet” (71). Proving causality is still a big hurdle in human studies (72,73). Some new studies have even suggested that functional MRI might not be as useful for understanding individual-level differences as previously thought (74). In essence, we do not yet have the technology or the intellectual capital to thoroughly understand the basis of social cognition. However, none of these setbacks mean that it is impossible to answer these questions or that we should give up.

We must recognize that neuroscience has made enormous progress in less than a century. Just 70 years ago, Hodkin and Huxley were still sticking electrodes into squids and figuring out how neurons work (75). Now, we have reached a point where we have sophisticated methods of observing, perturbing, and modeling nervous systems at a scale that would have been almost unimaginable then. With that in mind, who knows what we will be capable of doing 50 years from now?

Neuropolitics has a lot more to offer us. Like Schrieber wrote in a recent review, “the ‘friendship’ between neuroscience and politics is still in the acquaintance stage” (52). I would like to conclude this essay with a quote from Robert Heinlein that explains the potential this field has in one sentence: “Everything is theoretically impossible until it is done.”

References:

1.     Bausells M. Our Existence Is Political: An Interview With JJ Bola, Author Of Word [Internet]. Electric Literature. 2015 [cited 2021 Apr 14]. Available from: https://electricliterature.com/our-existence-is-political-an-interview-with-jj-bola-author-of-word/

2.     Bhargava R. Political Theory: An Introduction. Pearson Education India; 2008. 358 p.

3.     Robinson H. Dualism. In: Zalta EN, editor. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy [Internet]. Fall 2020. Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University; 2020 [cited 2021 Apr 14]. Available from: https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2020/entries/dualism/

4.     Rilling JK, King-Casas B, Sanfey AG. The neurobiology of social decision-making. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2008 Apr;18(2):159–65.

5.     Rilling J, Sanfey A. The neuroscience of social decision-making. Annu Rev Psychol. 2011;

6.     Sanfey AG. Social decision-making: insights from game theory and neuroscience. Science. 2007 Oct 26;318(5850):598–602.

7.     Haas IJ, Warren C, Lauf SJ. Political Neuroscience: Understanding How the Brain Makes Political Decisions [Internet]. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Politics. 2020 [cited 2021 Apr 14]. Available from: https://oxfordre.com/politics/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228637.001.0001/acrefore-9780190228637-e-948

8.     Sperry RW, Zaidel E, Zaidel D. Self recognition and social awareness in the deconnected minor hemisphere. Neuropsychologia. 1979 Jan 1;17(2):153–66.

9.     Cacioppo JT, Visser PS. Political Psychology and Social Neuroscience: Strange Bedfellows or Comrades in Arms? Polit Psychol. 2003;24(4):647–56.

10.     Lieberman MD, Schreiber D, Ochsner KN. Is Political Cognition Like Riding a Bicycle? How Cognitive Neuroscience Can Inform Research on Political Thinking. Polit Psychol. 2003;24(4):681–704.

11.     Schreiber DM. Evaluating politics: A search for the neural substrates of political thought [Internet] [Ph.D.]. [United States -- California]: University of California, Los Angeles; 2005 [cited 2021 Apr 14]. Available from: https://search.proquest.com/docview/305000724/abstract/C66FFA73AC7549B9PQ/1

12.     Jost JT, Amodio DM. Political ideology as motivated social cognition: Behavioral and neuroscientific evidence. Motiv Emot. 2012 Mar 1;36(1):55–64.

13.     Schreiber D. Political Cognition as Social Cognition: Are We All Political Sophisticates? 2007 Jan 1;

14.     Waal F de, Waal FBM. Chimpanzee Politics: Power and Sex Among Apes. JHU Press; 2007. 280 p.

15.     Humphrey N. The Social Function of Intellect. In: Growing Points in Ethology. 1976. p. 303–17.

16.     Fowler JH, Schreiber D. Biology, Politics, and the Emerging Science of Human Nature. Science. 2008 Nov 7;322(5903):912–4.

17.     Haas IJ. Chapter 19 - Political Neuroscience. In: Absher JR, Cloutier J, editors. Neuroimaging Personality, Social Cognition, and Character [Internet]. San Diego: Academic Press; 2016 [cited 2021 Apr 14]. p. 355–70. Available from: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128009352000191

18.     Moffett MW. The Human Swarm: How Our Societies Arise, Thrive, and Fall. Head of Zeus Ltd; 2019. 743 p.

19.     Amodio DM, Jost JT, Master SL, Yee CM. Neurocognitive correlates of liberalism and conservatism. Nat Neurosci. 2007 Oct;10(10):1246–7.

20.     Haas IJ, Baker MN, Gonzalez FJ. Who Can Deviate from the Party Line? Political Ideology Moderates Evaluation of Incongruent Policy Positions in Insula and Anterior Cingulate Cortex. Soc Justice Res. 2017 Dec 1;30(4):355–80.

21.     Kanai R, Feilden T, Firth C, Rees G. Political Orientations Are Correlated with Brain Structure in Young Adults. Curr Biol. 2011 Apr 26;21(8):677–80.

22.     Weissflog M, Choma B, Dywan J, van Noordt S, Segalowitz S. The political (and physiological) divide: Political orientation, performance monitoring, and the anterior cingulate response. Soc Neurosci. 2013 Sep 1;8:434–47.

23.     Jost JT, Banaji MR, Nosek BA. A Decade of System Justification Theory: Accumulated Evidence of Conscious and Unconscious Bolstering of the Status Quo. Polit Psychol. 2004;25(6):881–919.

24.     Jost JT, Sapolsky RM, Nam HH. Speculations on the Evolutionary Origins of System Justification. Evol Psychol. 2018 Apr 1;16(2):1474704918765342.

25.     Nam HH, Jost JT, Kaggen L, Campbell-Meiklejohn D, Van Bavel JJ. Amygdala structure and the tendency to regard the social system as legitimate and desirable. Nat Hum Behav. 2018 Feb;2(2):133–8.

26.     Nam HH. Neuroscientific approaches to the study of system justification. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2020 Aug 1;34:205–10.

27.     Krosch AR, Jost JT, Van Bavel JJ. The neural basis of ideological differences in race categorization. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2021 Apr 12;376(1822):20200139.

28.     Leong YC, Chen J, Willer R, Zaki J. Conservative and liberal attitudes drive polarized neural responses to political content. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2020 Nov 3;117(44):27731–9.

29.     Nam HH, Jost JT, Meager MR, Van Bavel JJ. Toward a neuropsychology of political orientation: exploring ideology in patients with frontal and midbrain lesions. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2021 Apr 12;376(1822):20200137.

30.     Yun JH, Kim Y, Lee E-J. ERP Study of Liberals’ and Conservatives’ Moral Reasoning Processes: Evidence from South Korea. J Bus Ethics [Internet]. 2021 Jan 26 [cited 2021 Apr 17]; Available from: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-021-04734-2

31.     Moore A, Hong S, Cram L. Trust in information, political identity and the brain: an interdisciplinary fMRI study. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2021 Apr 12;376(1822):20200140.

32.     Schreiber D, Fonzo G, Simmons AN, Dawes CT, Flagan T, Fowler JH, et al. Red Brain, Blue Brain: Evaluative Processes Differ in Democrats and Republicans. PLOS ONE. 2013 Feb 13;8(2):e52970.

33.     Dawes CT, Loewen PJ, Schreiber D, Simmons AN, Flagan T, McElreath R, et al. Neural basis of egalitarian behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2012 Apr 24;109(17):6479–83.

34.     Weissflog M, Choma BL, Dywan J. SOCIOPOLITICAL IDEOLOGY AND ELECTROCORTICAL RESPONSE S. undefined [Internet]. 2010 [cited 2021 Apr 14]; Available from: /paper/SOCIOPOLITICAL-IDEOLOGY-AND-ELECTROCORTICAL-S-Weissflog-Choma/aa14e1998f40416a9582b9637bf41ac7e8faaa3c

35.     Pedersen WS, Muftuler LT, Larson CL. Conservatism and the neural circuitry of threat: economic conservatism predicts greater amygdala–BNST connectivity during periods of threat vs safety. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2018 Jan 1;13(1):43–51.

36.     Inzlicht M, McGregor I, Hirsh JB, Nash K. Neural Markers of Religious Conviction. Psychol Sci. 2009 Mar 1;20(3):385–92.

37.     Kremláček J, Musil D, Langrová J, Palecek M. Neural Correlates of Liberalism and Conservatism in a Post-communist Country. Front Hum Neurosci [Internet]. 2019 [cited 2021 Apr 14];13. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00119/full#B38

38.     Wendell D. Neural Correlates of Political Attitudes: Emotion and Ideology in the Brain. 2016.

39.     Jost JT, Noorbaloochi S, Van Bavel JJ. The “chicken-and-egg” problem in political neuroscience. Behav Brain Sci. 2014 Jun;37(3):317–8.

40.     Voss P, Thomas ME, Cisneros-Franco JM, de Villers-Sidani É. Dynamic Brains and the Changing Rules of Neuroplasticity: Implications for Learning and Recovery. Front Psychol [Internet]. 2017 Oct 4 [cited 2021 Apr 14];8. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5649212/

41.     Spezio ML, Rangel A, Alvarez RM, O’Doherty JP, Mattes K, Todorov A, et al. A neural basis for the effect of candidate appearance on election outcomes. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2008 Dec;3(4):344–52.

42.     Tusche A, Kahnt T, Wisniewski D, Haynes J-D. Automatic processing of political preferences in the human brain. NeuroImage. 2013 Jan 24;72.

43.     Ballew CC, Todorov A. Predicting political elections from rapid and unreflective face judgments. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2007 Nov 13;104(46):17948–53.

44.     Olivola CY, Todorov A. Elected in 100 milliseconds: Appearance-based trait inferences and voting. J Nonverbal Behav. 2010;34(2):83–110.

45.     Coronel JC, Duff MC, Warren DE, Federmeier KD, Gonsalves BD, Tranel D, et al. Remembering and Voting: Theory and Evidence from Amnesic Patients. Am J Polit Sci. 2012 Oct;56(4):837–48.

46.     Kaplan JT, Freedman J, Iacoboni M. Us versus them: Political attitudes and party affiliation influence neural response to faces of presidential candidates. Neuropsychologia. 2007 Jan 7;45(1):55–64.

47.     Kaplan JT, Gimbel SI, Harris S. Neural correlates of maintaining one’s political beliefs in the face of counterevidence. Sci Rep. 2016 Dec 23;6(1):39589.

48.     Westen D, Blagov PS, Harenski K, Kilts C, Hamann S. Neural bases of motivated reasoning: an FMRI study of emotional constraints on partisan political judgment in the 2004 U.S. Presidential election. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006 Nov;18(11):1947–58.

49.     Sánchez-Fernández J, Casado-Aranda L-A. Neural Predictors of Changes in Party Closeness after Exposure to Corruption Messages: An fMRI Study. Brain Sci. 2021 Feb;11(2):158.

50.     Zmigrod L, Tsakiris M. Computational and neurocognitive approaches to the political brain: key insights and future avenues for political neuroscience. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2021 Apr 12;376(1822):20200130.

51.     Mattan BD, Wei KY, Cloutier J, Kubota JT. The social neuroscience of race-based and status-based prejudice. Curr Opin Psychol. 2018 Dec 1;24:27–34.

52.     Schreiber D. Neuropolitics: Twenty years later. Polit Life Sci J Assoc Polit Life Sci. 2017;36(2):114–31.

53.     Lieberman MD, Hariri A, Jarcho JM, Eisenberger NI, Bookheimer SY. An fMRI investigation of race-related amygdala activity in African-American and Caucasian-American individuals. Nat Neurosci. 2005 Jun;8(6):720–2.

54.     Phelps E, O’Connor K, Cunningham W, Funayama E, Gatenby C, Gore J, et al. Performance on Indirect Measures of Race Evaluation Predicts Amygdala Activity. J Cogn Neurosci. 2000 Oct 1;12:729–38.

55.     Richeson JA, Baird AA, Gordon HL, Heatherton TF, Wyland CL, Trawalter S, et al. An fMRI investigation of the impact of interracial contact on executive function. Nat Neurosci. 2003 Dec;6(12):1323–8.

56.     Amodio DM, Harmon-Jones E, Devine PG, Curtin JJ, Hartley SL, Covert AE. Neural Signals for the Detection of Unintentional Race Bias. Psychol Sci. 2004 Feb 1;15(2):88–93.

57.     Amodio DM, Kubota JT, Harmon-Jones E, Devine PG. Alternative mechanisms for regulating racial responses according to internal vs external cues. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2006 Jun 1;1(1):26–36.

58.     Ito TA, Urland GR. The influence of processing objectives on the perception of faces: An ERP study of race and gender perception. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2005 Mar 1;5(1):21–36.

59.     Ronquillo J, Denson TF, Lickel B, Lu Z-L, Nandy A, Maddox KB. The effects of skin tone on race-related amygdala activity: an fMRI investigation. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2007 Mar 1;2(1):39–44.

60.     Kubota JT, Banaji MR, Phelps EA. The neuroscience of race. Nat Neurosci. 2012 Jul;15(7):940–8.

61.     Sapolsky RM. Behave: The Biology of Humans at Our Best and Worst. Penguin; 2017. 948 p.

62.     Sapolsky R. This Is Your Brain on Nationalism. 2020 Aug 31 [cited 2021 Apr 14]; Available from: https://www.foreignaffairs.com/articles/2019-02-12/your-brain-nationalism

63.     Takeuchi H, Taki Y, Sekiguchi A, Nouchi R, Kotozaki Y, Nakagawa S, et al. Differences in gray matter structure correlated to nationalism and patriotism. Sci Rep [Internet]. 2016 Jul 15 [cited 2021 Apr 14];6. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4945903/

64.     Fowler JH, Dawes CT. Two Genes Predict Voter Turnout. J Polit. 2008 Jul 1;70(3):579–94.

65.     Fowler JH, Baker LA, Dawes CT. Genetic Variation in Political Participation. Am Polit Sci Rev. 2008 May;102(2):233–48.

66.     Harder J, Krosnick J. Why Do People Vote? A Psychological Analysis of the Causes of Voter Turnout. J Soc Issues. 2008 Aug 5;64:525–49.

67.     Gozzi M, Zamboni G, Krueger F, Grafman J. Interest in politics modulates neural activity in the amygdala and ventral striatum. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010 Nov;31(11):1763–71.

68.     Bischoff I, Neuhaus C, Trautner P, Weber B. The neuroeconomics of voting: Neural evidence of different sources of utility in voting. J Neurosci Psychol Econ. 2013;6(4):215–35.

69.     Aron A, Badre D, Brett M, Cacioppo J, Chambers C, Cools R, et al. Opinion | Politics and the Brain. The New York Times [Internet]. 2007 Nov 14 [cited 2021 Apr 14]; Available from: https://www.nytimes.com/2007/11/14/opinion/lweb14brain.html

70.     Iacoboni M, Freedman J, Kaplan J. Opinion | This Is Your Brain on Politics. The New York Times [Internet]. 2007 Nov 11 [cited 2021 Apr 14]; Available from: https://www.nytimes.com/2007/11/11/opinion/11freedman.html

71.     Slaby J. Neuroscience and Politics: Do Not Hold Your Breath [Internet]. E-International Relations. 2015 [cited 2021 Apr 14]. Available from: https://www.e-ir.info/2015/05/08/neuroscience-and-politics-do-not-hold-your-breath/

72.     Etkin A. Addressing the Causality Gap in Human Psychiatric Neuroscience. JAMA Psychiatry. 2018 Jan 1;75(1):3–4.

73.     Weichwald S, Peters J. Causality in Cognitive Neuroscience: Concepts, Challenges, and Distributional Robustness. J Cogn Neurosci. 2021 Feb;33(2):226–47.

74.     Elliott ML, Knodt AR, Ireland D, Morris ML, Poulton R, Ramrakha S, et al. What Is the Test-Retest Reliability of Common Task-Functional MRI Measures? New Empirical Evidence and a Meta-Analysis. Psychol Sci. 2020 Jul 1;31(7):792–806.

75.     Hodgkin AL, Huxley AF. A quantitative description of membrane current and its application to conduction and excitation in nerve. J Physiol. 1952 Aug 28;117(4):500–44.

Rishika Mohanta

Rishika (She/her) identifies as a pan-romantic trans woman and is an active researcher in the field of neuroscience and a student at IISER Pune. She is an aspiring queer/trans activist and an intersectional feminist. Find out more about her on Twitter @NeuroRishika.